Mass media takes up a large part of our leisure time: people spend an average of 25 hours a week watching TV, and they also have time for radio, movies, magazines and newspapers. For kids, time spent watching TV is similar to time spent at school or with family and friends. While school, family and friends are all cited as major factors influencing children's social interactions, there has been a lively debate around the possible influence of mass media, with results both in favor and against. Impact questions are often raised out of the urgency of the public rather than academic agenda, the simplicity of which does not lend itself to the complexity of the problem (we don't ask for other social impacts, what is the parent's impact on the child? What is the school's impact on the family? What positive or negative influence does a friend have. The possibility of a media effect is often seen as a challenge to personal respect and autonomy, just as the pro-effect view sees the public as a gullible mass, cultural idiots, vulnerable to ideological injection needle harm, as if television was proposed as the sole cause of a range of social behaviors. This stereotyped view of research tends to foster an equally stereotyped alternative view that creative and informed viewers make rational choices about what to watch. <br>Review Articles often describe the history of research progress over the past 70 years, alternating between these two extremes—first we believe in strong effects, then arguments for zero effects, then regression to strong effects, etc.—when reinterpreting old ones with new eyes A history of contradictions becoming apparent when researching. Contemporary media research sometimes defines itself by rejecting the study of effect language—criticism of laboratory experiments, causal reasoning logic, and psychological reductionism. As I will argue in this chapter, this rejection is partly justified Yes, some are exaggerated
正在翻译中..